题名

華語捲舌音對比的聽覺辨識與頻譜動差分析

并列篇名

The Auditory Discrimination of Mandarin Retroflex Contrasts and Spectral Moment Analysis

DOI

10.6129/CJP.2009.5102.02

作者

鄭靜宜(Jing-Yi Jeng)

关键词

M1 ; 動差分析 ; 捲舌音 ; 頻譜 ; 聽覺辨識 ; auditory recognition ; Mandarin fricatives ; moments ; spectral retroflex

期刊名称

中華心理學刊

卷期/出版年月

51卷2期(2009 / 06 / 01)

页次

157 - 173

内容语文

繁體中文

中文摘要

There are three pairs of retroflex contrasts in Mandarin fricative and affricates, and perceptually, they are very similar and not easy to be distinguished, especially for non-skilled listeners. The purpose of this study is to investigate the auditory perception of retroflexation of Mandarin in adults and investigate the relationship between the spectral moments of frication noise and the auditory discrimination of the fricatives and affricates with retroflexed contrasts. There were thirty adults of native Mandarin speakers participating in the retroflex recognition tasks. There were two tasks of retroflexation judgment in the experiment. In the first one task, the monosyllabic words were used as stimuli and the listeners were asked to judge the retroflexation of Mandarin monosyllabic words. In the second task, the disyllabic words were used, and the retroflexation of the first syllables of words was required to be judged. In the first task, the monosyllable stimuli had three degrees of retroflexation variations which was consciously made by the speakers, including nonretroflexation, slightly retroflexation, and more retroflexation. The results show that if the slightly retroflexed stimuli were excluded, the correctness percentage of responds was 78% for all listeners. The highly retroflexed consonants had the significantly shortest reaction time (RT), while the RTs between the nonretroflexed and the slightly retroflexed had no significant difference. According to the retroflexness categorical responses of the listeners, the spectral moment analysis of the stimuli showed that the retroflexed consonants had significantly lower M1, higher M2, higher M3, and lower M4, compared to the nonretroflxed consonants. The results are compatible with the data of the previous study of retroflex production (Jeng, 2005). Among the four spectral moments, M1 was the most robust parameter for discriminating the retroflex contrasts The correlation coefficient between the retroflex response percentages and M1 of the stimuli was -0.61 (p<.0001). In the second task, the listeners were asked to judge the retroflexation of the first syllables of the disyllabic words based on their acoustical retroflexation, instead of their lexical retroflexation. However the results show that the retroflexation judgment of the listeners was inevitably influenced by the lexical information of the second syllables of the words. Among the four spectral moments, M1 still the most robust parameter for discriminating the retroflex contrasts. The interaction of the lexical information and acoustical information during the retroflexation judgment of the disyllabic words was discussed.

英文摘要

There are three pairs of retroflex contrasts in Mandarin fricative and affricates, and perceptually, they are very similar and not easy to be distinguished, especially for non-skilled listeners. The purpose of this study is to investigate the auditory perception of retroflexation of Mandarin in adults and investigate the relationship between the spectral moments of frication noise and the auditory discrimination of the fricatives and affricates with retroflexed contrasts. There were thirty adults of native Mandarin speakers participating in the retroflex recognition tasks. There were two tasks of retroflexation judgment in the experiment. In the first one task, the monosyllabic words were used as stimuli and the listeners were asked to judge the retroflexation of Mandarin monosyllabic words. In the second task, the disyllabic words were used, and the retroflexation of the first syllables of words was required to be judged. In the first task, the monosyllable stimuli had three degrees of retroflexation variations which was consciously made by the speakers, including nonretroflexation, slightly retroflexation, and more retroflexation. The results show that if the slightly retroflexed stimuli were excluded, the correctness percentage of responds was 78% for all listeners. The highly retroflexed consonants had the significantly shortest reaction time (RT), while the RTs between the nonretroflexed and the slightly retroflexed had no significant difference. According to the retroflexness categorical responses of the listeners, the spectral moment analysis of the stimuli showed that the retroflexed consonants had significantly lower M1, higher M2, higher M3, and lower M4, compared to the nonretroflxed consonants. The results are compatible with the data of the previous study of retroflex production (Jeng, 2005). Among the four spectral moments, M1 was the most robust parameter for discriminating the retroflex contrasts The correlation coefficient between the retroflex response percentages and M1 of the stimuli was -0.61 (p<.0001). In the second task, the listeners were asked to judge the retroflexation of the first syllables of the disyllabic words based on their acoustical retroflexation, instead of their lexical retroflexation. However the results show that the retroflexation judgment of the listeners was inevitably influenced by the lexical information of the second syllables of the words. Among the four spectral moments, M1 still the most robust parameter for discriminating the retroflex contrasts. The interaction of the lexical information and acoustical information during the retroflexation judgment of the disyllabic words was discussed.

主题分类 社會科學 > 心理學
参考文献
  1. Praat
  2. Forrest, K.,Weismer, G.,Elbert, M.,Dinnsen, D. A.(1994).Spectral analysis of target-appropriate /t/ and /k/ produced by phonologically disordered and normally articulating children.Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics,8,267-281.
  3. Forrest, K.,Weismer, G.,Hodge, M.,Dinnsen, D. A.,Elbert, M.(1990).Statistical analysis of word-initial /k/ and /t/ produced by normal and phonologically disordered children.Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics,4,327-340.
  4. Forrest, K.,Weismer, G.,Milenkovic, P.,Dougall, R.(1988).Statistical analysis of word-initial voiceless obstruents: Preliminary data.Journal of Acoustical Society of America,84,115-123.
  5. Forster, K. I.,Forster, J. C.(2004).DMDX, ver 3.Monash University and University of Arizona.
  6. International Phonetic Alphabet
  7. Jeng, J. Y.(2000).Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Wisconsin-Madison.
  8. Jongman, A.,Wayland, R.,Wong, S.(2000).Acoustic characteristics of English fricatives.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,108,1252-1263.
  9. Kent, R. D.,Read, C.(2002).The acoustic analysis of speech.San Diego:Singular.
  10. Klatt, D. H.(1979).Speech perception: A model of acoustic-phonetic analysis and lexical access.Journal of Phonetics,7,279-312.
  11. Ladefoged, P.,Wu, Z. J.(1984).Places of articulation: An investigation of Pekingese fricatives and affricates.Journal of Phonetics,2,267-278.
  12. Milenkovic, P.(2004).TF32 [Computer Program].Madison, WI:University of Wisconsin-Madison, Department of Electrical Engineering.
  13. Milenkovic, P.(1999).Analysis and synthesis of unvoiced speech using spectrum moments.Workshop on spectrum moments measures for speech language and hearing research,San Francisco:
  14. Moments: Batch speech spectrum moments analysis
  15. Nowak, P. M.(2006).The role of vowel transitions and frication noise in the perception of Polish sibilants.Journal of Phonetics,34,139-152.
  16. Oden, G. C.,Marssaro, D. W.(1978).Integration of featural information in speech perception.Psychological Review,85,172-191.
  17. Pickett, J. M.(1999).The acoustics of speech communication: Fundamentals, speech perception theory, and technology.Boston:Allyn and Bacon.
  18. Shriberg, L. D.,Kent, R. D.,Karlsson, H. B.,McSweeny, J. L.,Nadler, C. J.,Brown, R. L.(2003).A diagnostic marker for speech delay associated with otitis media with effusion: Backing of obstruents.Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics,17,529-548.
  19. Stevens, K. N.(1998).Acoustic phonetics.Cambridge:MIT Press.
  20. Stevens, K. N.,P. B. Denes (Eds.),E. E. David (Eds.)(1972).Human communication: A unified view.New York:McGraw-Hill.
  21. 王南梅、費珮妮、黃恂、陳靜文(1987)。三歲至六歲學齡前兒童華語語音發展結構。聽語會刊,1,12-15。
  22. 注音符號補救教學之行動研究
  23. 翁秀民、沈牧璋、郭德惠(2006)。華語捲舌子音及其相對非捲舌子音的比較研究。高雄應用科技大學學報,35,325-333。
  24. 曹逢甫(2000)。台式日語與台灣國語-百年來在台灣發生的兩個語言接觸的實例。漢學研究,18,273-297。
  25. 鄭靜宜(2006)。國語捲舌音和非捲舌音的聲學特性。南大人文研究學報,40,27-48。
  26. 謝國平(1998)。臺灣地區年輕人ㄓㄔㄕ與ㄗㄘㄙ真的不分嗎?。華文世界,90,1-7。
被引用次数
  1. 黎旅揚、曹峰銘(2014)。低年級閱讀困難兒童輔音知覺及其與閱讀發展的關聯。中華心理學刊,56(1),97-116。
  2. 劉惠美、曹峰銘、張鑑如、徐儷玲(2013)。學前到學齡兒童的語音區辨能力發展及其與詞彙理解的關係 。教育心理學報,45(2),221-240。
  3. 鄭靜宜(2011)。學習障礙兒童對華語捲舌音特徵的聽知覺辨識。特殊教育研究學刊,36(2),27-50。
  4. 鄭靜宜(2016)。語音異常兒童的語音區辨及聲學調整對其聽知覺的影響。特殊教育研究學刊,41(3),35-66。
  5. 鄭尊仁(2022)。韓國學生的華語舌尖前後音偏誤考察:以頻譜重心為基礎。北市大語文學報,二十七,69-86。
  6. (2014).Mandarin Retroflex Sounds Perceived by Non-native Speakers.臺北教育大學語文集刊,26,39-76.