题名

16至20個月嬰幼兒對意圖線索的理解

并列篇名

16- to 20-Month-Old Understanding of Others' Intentional Cues

DOI

10.6129/CJP.2011.5302.02

作者

王瑾婷(Chin-Ting Wang);姜忠信(Chung-Hsin Chiang)

关键词

意圖理解 ; 語音線索 ; 情緒線索 ; 嬰幼兒 ; intentional understanding ; vocal cues ; emotional cues ; toddlers

期刊名称

中華心理學刊

卷期/出版年月

53卷2期(2011 / 06 / 01)

页次

149 - 166

内容语文

繁體中文

中文摘要

本研究探討16個月及20個月大的嬰幼兒,能否依語音和表情線索,來區辨他人行為的意圖。首先,透過半結構化親子互動遊戲,引發家長發出語音,企圖找出可與”there”及”woops”對應之中文的意圖性及意外性語音,前導實驗中以18個月與24個月的嬰幼兒及其家長為對象,結果發現,使用次數較多的是意圖性語音「ㄛ」和意外性語音「(特殊字元略)」。實驗一重製姜忠信與張閔翔(2008)的實驗,修正語音及降低作業難度。結果發現,20個月大的嬰幼兒大體上能依語音及表情線索找到意圖目標物,16個月的嬰幼兒則否,兩年齡組嬰幼兒在作答上,皆無明顯的左右偏好。實驗二再修正實驗一,移除打開盒蓋的動作來降低作業難度,另增加「最終反應」的計分。結果發現:20個月大的嬰幼兒能理解語音及表情的意圖線索,亦發現最終反應顯著高於第一反應,說明此月齡的嬰幼兒需較長的時間,才能做出正確反應。然而實驗二嬰幼兒的表現與實驗一20個月大的嬰幼兒無異,說明了盒蓋並非造成作業難度較高的主因。由本研究結果,推論20個月大的嬰幼兒,能夠理解他人語音及表情的意圖線索。

英文摘要

Recent literature has found that young children before the age of 2 can use behavioral cues to detect others' intentions. The present study used vocal and facial expression cues to investigate intentional ability in 16- to 20-monthold children. A pilot study looked at the Mandarin vocals that are synonymous with ”there” and ”woops” in English. In a semi-structured parent-and-child interaction context, 18- and 24-month-old children played with their parents. The parents' vocals in intentional and accidental conditions were recorded. Results showed that the intentional vocal ”Ah!” and accidental vocal ”Wa!” were most frequently used. In Study 1, we replicated the referential intent task (Chiang & Chang, 2008), corrected the vocal cues, and reduced the task difficulty. The examiner provided vocal and facial expression cues to indicate which container contained the toy. Results showed that 20-month-old children could differentiate between the demonstrated intentional and accidental cues. The 16-month-old children did not pass the task. The preference to choose the right or left side was not significant in either age group. Study 2 removed the lid of containers to reduce task difficulty, coding children's first response and last response. Similarly, 20-month-old children passed the referential intent task. Performance on the last response was better than performance on the first response, and these children needed longer time to process information. However, the children in Study 2 did not perform better than those in Study 1. This result explained that the lid of the container did not decrease the task demand. In conclusion, 20-month-old children can understand others' intentional cues of vocal and facial expression. However, further studies are needed to explore the components of task loading and compare the data with that in Western culture.

主题分类 社會科學 > 心理學
参考文献
  1. 姜忠信、張閔翔(2008)。幼兒理解他人意圖的能力:語言及表情的線索。中華心理學刊,50,241-256。
    連結:
  2. Baldwin, D. A.(1993).Early referential understanding: Infants' ability to recognize referential acts for what they are.Developmental Psychology,29,832-843.
  3. Behne, T.,Carpenter, M.,Tomasello, M.(2005).One-year-olds comprehend the communicative intentions behind gestures in a hiding game.Developmental Science,8,492-499.
  4. Buresh, J. S.,Woodward, A. L.(2007).Infants track action goals within and across agents.Cognition,104,287-314.
  5. Carpenter, M.,Akhtar, N.,Tomasello, M.(1998).Fourteen through 18-months-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental action.Infant Behavior and Development,21,315-330.
  6. Corkum, V. L.,Moore, C.(1995).Development of joint visual attention in infants.Joint attention: Its origins and role in development,Hillsdale, NJ:
  7. Corkum, V. L.,Moore, C.(1998).The origins of joint visual attention in infants.Developmental psychology,34,28-38.
  8. Gergely, G.,Nadasdy, Z.,Csibra, G.,Biro, S.(1995).Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age.Cognition,56,165-193.
  9. Gräfenhain, M.,Behne, T.,Carpenter, M.,Tomasello, M.(2009).One-year-olds' understanding of nonverbal gestures directed to a third person.Cognitive Development,24,23-33.
  10. Huang, C. T.,Heyes, C.,Charman, T.(2002).Infants' behavioral reenactment of "failed attempts": Exploring the roles of emulation learning, stimulus enhancement, and understanding of intention.Developmental Psychology,38,840-855.
  11. Meltzoff, A. N.(2009).Roots of social cognition: The like-me framework.Minnesota symposia on child psychology: Meeting the challenge of translational research in child psychology,Hoboken, NJ:
  12. Meltzoff, A. N.(1995).Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children.Developmental Psychology,31,838-850.
  13. Moore, C.,Corkum, V.(1998).Infant gaze following based on eye direction.British Journal of Developmental Psychology,16,495-503.
  14. Mullen, E. M.(1995).Mullen scales of early learning: AGS edition.Circle Pines, MN:American Guidance Service.
  15. Mumme, D. L.,Femald, A.,Herrera, C.(1996).Infants' responses to vocal and facial expressions in a social referencing paradigm.Child Development,67,3219-3237.
  16. Olineck, K. M.,Poulin-Dubois, D.(2005).Infants' ability to distinguish between intentional and accidental actions and its relation to internal state language.Infancy,8,91-100.
  17. Phillips, A. T.,Wellman, H. M.,Spelke, E. S.(2002).Infants' ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action.Cognition,85,53-78.
  18. Quintana, S. M.,Aboud, F. E.,Chao, R. K.,Contreras-Grau, J.,Cross, W. E., Jr.,Hudley, C.(2006).Race, ethnicity, and culture in child development: Contemporary research and future directions.Child Development,77,1129-1141.
  19. Repacholi, B.(1998).Infants' use of attentional cues to identify the referent of another person's emotional expression.Developmental Psychology,34,1017-1025.
  20. Repacholi, B.,Meltzoff, A.(2007).Emotional eavesdropping: Infants selectively respond to indirect emotional signals.Child Development,78,503-521.
  21. Tardif, T.,Wellman, H. M.,Fung, Y. F.,Liu, D.,Fang, F.(2005).Preschoolers' understanding of knowing-that and knowing-how in the United States and Hong Kong.Developmental Psychology,41,562-573.
  22. Tomasello, M.(1995).Joint attention as social cognition.Joint attention: Its origins and role in development,Hillsdale, NJ:
  23. Tomasello, M.,Carpenter, M.,Call, J.,Behne, T.,Moll, H.(2005).Understanding and sharing intentions: The ontogeny and phylogeny of cultural cognition.Behavioral and Brain Sciences,28,675-691.
  24. Woodward, A. L.(1999).Infants' ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors.Infant Beahvior and Development,22,145-160.
  25. Woodward, A. L.(1998).Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach.Cognition,69,1-34.
  26. Woodward, A. L.(2003).Infants' developing understanding of the link between looker and object.Developmental Science,6,297-311.
  27. Zelazo, P. D.(ed.),Astington, J. W.(ed.),Olson, D. R.(ed.)(1999).Developing theories of intention: Social understanding and self-control.Mahwah, NJ:Erlbaum.
  28. 姜忠信(2000)。國立台灣大學心理學研究所=National Taiwan University。
  29. 徐澄清、蘇喜、蕭淑貞、林家青、宋維村、張珏(1978)。學齡前兒童行為發展量表之修訂及初步常模之建立。中華民國小兒科醫學會雜誌,19,142-157。
被引用次数
  1. 陳紹慶(2013)。體現認知的心理學觀點及研究取向。中華心理學刊,55(3),405-416。
  2. 鍾宛玲、黃啟泰(2015)。前置意圖與因果理解對幼兒觀察學習他人錯誤的影響。中華心理學刊,57(3),213-227。
  3. 鍾宛玲、黃啟泰(2017)。二歲幼兒在工具使用情境下的觀察學習:模仿與仿效。中華心理學刊,59(1),29-44。