英文摘要
|
Electronic products, such as televisions, smartphones, tablets, and computers, have become an integral part of our lives. Not only are these items increasingly popular but they have also changed our daily routines and communication habits. The Common Sense census (Rideout, 2017) in the United States revealed that more than 90% of children had at least one electronic product at home. In Taiwan, Wei and Chuang (2016) sampled 876 preschool children, revealing that more than 90% were exposed to electronic products and approximately 10% had started to use electronic products before the age of 2. The effect of electronic product use on children's development is a concern for parents, teachers, and researchers in the field of early childhood education. However, studies on the relationship between electronic product use and children's development have produced mixed results. This study investigated (1) the use of electronic products by 3-year-old children in Taiwan, (2) whether children from different backgrounds spent different amounts of time using electronic products, and (3) how children's usage of electronic product influences their motor, language, cognitive-attention/executive function, and emotional development. The participants in this study were 2,164 children born in Taiwan between April 1, 2013 and March 31, 2014, comprising 1,113 boys (51.4%) and 1,051 girls (48.6%). The age range of the parents in the sample was predominantly 31-40 years (59.7% of fathers, 69.8% of mothers), followed by 41-50 years (31.1% of fathers, 15.9% of mothers). This study used the Physical Movement Development Questionnaire, Language Development Questionnaire, Cognitive Development Questionnaire (Attention/Executive Function), Social Emotional Development Questionnaire, and Electronic Product Use Questionnaire developed by the Kids in Taiwan: National, Longitudinal Study of Child Development and Care project (Chang, 2019) to collect the data. The SPSS and AMOS software programs were used to perform descriptive and inferential statistics and structural equation model analyses, respectively. Seven main results were obtained from this study: (1) Only 6.8% of the children had never used electronic products. Television was the most frequently used electronic product (78%), followed by smartphones (64.8%); the least used were video game consoles (2.8%) and notebook computers (4.1%). (2) Children spent more time watching television than using other electronic products daily. On average, more than half the children (57.3%) watched television within 1.5 hours per day, and 24.8% watched television for more than 2 hours per day. More than 70% used smartphones, tablets, notebook computers, and video game consoles within 1 hour. (3) The average time boys and girls spent watching television was 102 minutes and 90 minutes, respectively, and the difference was statistically significant. No significant difference, however, was identified for the time spent using smartphones, tablets, notebook computers, or video game consoles between boys and girls. (4) In this study, the children's places of residence were divided into "northern," "central," "southern," and "eastern." The results demonstrated that no significant difference existed in the time children spent watching television in terms of place of residence (F = 1.47, p > .05), but a significant difference was observed in the time children spent using smartphones, tablets, notebook computers, and video games with regard to place of residence (F = 6.34, p < .001). Children living in central (M = 45.76) and southern Taiwan (M = 47.17) spent significantly more time using smartphones, tablets, notebook computers, and video games than those living in northern Taiwan (M = 37.53). (5) A significant difference was identified in time spent watching television and using smartphones, tablets, notebook computers, and video games in relation to parents' education levels. Children whose parents had not completed high school (M= 112.37) watched television and used smartphones, tablets, computers, and video games for significantly longer periods than those whose parents had graduated from university or college (M = 93.89) or held a master's degree or above (M = 65.63). (6) Children with different household incomes spent different lengths of time watching television and using smartphones, tablets, computers, and video games. Children with a monthly household income under NT$40,000 spent significantly more time watching television than those whose monthly family income ranged from NT$70,000 to NT$120,000 or was NT$120,000 and above; children with a monthly family income between NT$40,000 and NT$70,000 watched television significantly more than those whose monthly household income was between NT$70,000 and NT$120,000 or NT$120,000 and above. In addition, children with a monthly household income under NT$40,000 used smartphones, tablets, computers, and video games the longest. (7) In terms of children's development, this study found that the children who spent the most time watching television (r = -.107, p < .01) and using smartphones, tablets, computers, and video games (r = -.095, p < .01) had a significantly lower level of fine motor development. A similar result was identified for language development. The correlation coefficients for the relationship between children's time spent watching television and their performance in receptive language, productive language, and emergent literacy were -.043 (p < .05), -.130 (p < .01), and -.138 (p < .01), respectively. As for the relationship between the time children spent using smartphones, tablets, computers, and video games and the three constructs of children's language development, the correlation coefficients all reached significant levels (receptive language: r = -.043, p < .05; productive language: r = -.136, p < .01; emergent literacy: r = -.130, p < .01). The results also demonstrated a significantly negative correlation between the time children spent watching television and their attention/executive function (r = -.129, p < .01). The same finding was evident in the relationship between the time children spent using smartphones, tablets, computers, and video games and their attention/executive function performance (r = -.100, p < .01). With respect to children's emotional development, similar results were observed. Significantly negative relationships were identified between the time children spent watching television and children's emotional awareness (r = -.062, p < .01), emotional expression (r = -.063, p < .01), and emotional understanding (r = -.063, p < .01). The results of the structural equation model analyses demonstrated that children's socioeconomic status (SES) could explain the time children spent using electronic products; the higher a child's SES was, the less time they spent using these products. The time children spent using electronic products was also linked to their level of development; the longer a child spent using these products, the worse their developmental level was. SES did not have a direct influence on children's development. In conclusion, this study extended previous research and revealed that the electronic products used most frequently by 3-year-old children in Taiwan were televisions and smartphones and the average time children spent watching television was higher than that recommended by the American Academy of Pediatrics. Significant differences were observed in the time children spent using electronic products when children's gender, parental education level, family income, and place of residence were analyzed. Significant negative relationships were also revealed between the time children spent using electronic products and their motor, language, cognitive, and emotional development, which is consistent with previous studies. The results of this study suggest that the prolonged use of electronic products has a negative impact on the development of 3-year-old children in Taiwan. Parents, caregivers, and educators should pay more attention to the time children spend using these products.
|
参考文献
|
-
王馨敏, S.,李俊仁, J.-R.,張鑑如, C.-J.(2015)。「二至五歲幼兒認知發展家長問卷」之編製。測驗學刊,62(4),279-302。
連結:
-
呂信慧, H.-H.,曹峰銘, F.-M.(2018)。遲語兒幼兒期至學齡前期的語言發展型態:兩年縱貫研究。教育心理學報,50(4),685-705。
連結:
-
林佳慧, C.-H.,劉惠美, H.-M.,張鑑如, C.-J.(2019)。家庭脈絡下的親子共讀與幼兒發展關係—臺灣幼兒發展調查資料庫的應用與分析。教育心理學報,51(1),135-159。
連結:
-
張鑑如, C.-J.,謝淑惠, S.-H.,周麗端, L.-T.,廖鳳瑞, F.-R.(2017)。幼兒發展調查資料庫建置計畫簡介。中國統計學報,55,42-62。
連結:
-
張鑑如, C.-J.,聶西平, H.-P.,周麗端, L.-T.(2018)。當代臺灣幼兒家庭基本資料、生活環境樣貌及能力發展:KIT 資料庫樣本。人類發展與家庭學報,19,45-63。
連結:
-
程景琳, C.-L.,涂妙如, M.-J.,陳虹仰, H.-Y.,張鑑如, C.-J.(2016)。學齡前嬰幼兒之社會情緒能力與嬰幼兒語言能力及父母教養之關聯。當代教育研究季刊,24(3),1-27。
連結:
-
劉惠美, H.-M.,陳柏熹, P.-H.,黃秀琦, S.-C.,陳雅雯, Y.-W.,張鑑如, C.-J.(2018)。「零至六歲嬰幼兒語言發展家長問卷」之編製。測驗學刊,65(1),95-123。
連結:
-
兒童及少年福利與權益保障法(2021 年 1 月 20 日)修正公布。https://law.moj.gov.tw/LawClass/LawAll.aspx?PCode=D0050001 [The Protection of Children and Youths Welfare and Rights Act. (2021, January 20). Amendment to Articles. https://law.moj.gov.tw/LawClass/LawAll.aspx?PCode=D0050001]
-
Chang, C.-J.,Luo, Y.-H.,Wu, R.(2016).Origins of print concepts at home: Print referencing during joint book reading interactions in Taiwanese mothers and children.Early Education and Development,27(1),54-73.
-
Chonchaiya, W.,Pruksananonda, C.(2008).Television viewing associates with delayed language development.Acta Paediatrica,97(7),977-982.
-
Cicchetti, D.(Ed.),Cohen, D. J.(Ed.)(1995).Manual of developmental psychopathology: Theory and methods.Wiley.
-
Clay, M. M.(1966).University of Auckland.
-
Cox, R.,Skouteris, H.,Rutherford, L.,Fuller-Tyszkiewicz, M.,Dell, D.,Hardy, L. L.(2012).Television viewing, television content, food intake, physical activity and body mass index: A cross-sectional study of preschool children aged 2-6 years.Health Promotion Journal of Australia,23(1),58-62.
-
Eisenberg, N.,Guthrie, I. K.,Fabes, R. A.,Reiser, M.,Murphy, B. C.,Holgren, R.,Maszk, P.,Losoya, S.(1997).The relations of regulation and emotionality to resiliency and competent social functioning in elementary school children.Child Development,68(2),295-311.
-
Gallahue、 D. L.,許義雄(譯), I.-H.(Trans.)(2014).兒童發展與身體教育.復文書局=Fu Wen Books.
-
Gleason, J. B.,Ratner, N. B.(2017).The development of language.Pearson Education.
-
Hohm, E.,Jennen-Steinmetz, C.,Schmidt, M. H.,Laucht, M.(2007).Language development at ten months: Predictive of language outcome and school achievement ten years later?.European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,16(3),149-156.
-
Hu, L.,Bentler, P. M.(1999).Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives.Structural Equation Modeling,6(1),1-55.
-
Kamenetz, A. (2016). American Academy of Pediatrics lifts ‘ no screens under 2’ rule. NPR. https://www.npr.org/sections/ed/2016/10/21/498550475/american-academy-of-pediatrics-lifts-no-screens-under-2-rule
-
Lightfoot, C.,Cole, M.,Cole, S. R.(2009).The development of children.Worth.
-
Lin, L-Y.,Cherng, R-J.,Chen, Y.-J.(2017).Effect of touch screen tablet use on fine motor development of young children.Physical and Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics,37(5),457-467.
-
McCartney, K.(Ed.),Phillips, D.(Ed.)(2008).Blackwell handbook of early childhood development.Blackwell.
-
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network(2005).Child care and child development: Results from the NICHD study of early child care and youth development.Guilford.
-
Page, A. S.,Cooper, A. R.,Griew, P.,Jago, R. P.(2010).Children’s screen viewing is related to physiological difficulties irrespective of physical activity.Pediatrics,126(5),Article e1017.
-
Parkes, A.,Sweeting, H.,Wight, D.,Henderson, M.(2013).Do television and electronic games predict children’s psychosocial adjustment? Longitudinal research using the UK Millennium Cohort Study.Archives of Disease in Childhood,98(5),341-348.
-
Posner, M. I.,Petersen, S. E.(1990).The attention system of the human brain.Annual Review of Neuroscience,13,25-42.
-
Radesky, J. S.,Peacock-Chambers, E.,Zuckerman, B.,Silverstein, M.(2016).Use of mobile technology to calm upset children: Associations with social-emotional development.JAMA Pediatrics,170(4),397-399.
-
Rideout, V. (2017). The Common Sense census: Media use by kids age zero to eight. Common Sense Media. https://www.commonsensemedia.org/sites/default/files/uploads/research/csm_zerotoeight_fullreport_release_2.pdf
-
Rideout, V., Saphir, M., Pai, S., Rudd, A. (2013). Zero to eight: Children’s media use in America 2013. Common Sense Media. https://www.commonsensemedia.org/zero-to-eight-2013-infographic
-
Salmon, J.,Timperio, A.,Telford, A.,Carver, A.,Crawford, D.(2005).Association of family environment with children’s television viewing and with low level of physical activity.Obesity Research,13(11),1939-1951.
-
Santrock, J. W.(2011).Life-span development.McGraw-Hill.
-
Schmidt, M. E.,Rich, M.,Rifas-Shiman, S. L.,Oken, E.,Taveras, E. M.(2009).Television viewing in infancy and child cognition at 3 years of age in a US cohort.Pediatrics,123(3),e370-e375.
-
Shaffer, D. R.(2008).Social and personality development.Wadsworth.
-
Zimmerman, F. J.,Christakis, D. A.,Meltzoff A. N.(2007).Associations between media viewing and language development in children under age 2 years.The Journal of Pediatrics,151(4),364-368.
-
李佳蓓, J.-P.(2015)。國立臺中教育大學=National Taichung University of Education。
-
李佩芬, P.-F.(2017)。國立臺中教育大學=National Taichung University of Education。
-
兒童福利聯盟(2012):〈2012 年兒童使用 3C 產品現況調查報告〉。https://www.children.org.tw/publication_research/research_report/2116 [Child Welfare League Foundation. (2012). 2012 Report on usage of 3C products in children. https://www.children.org.tw/publication_research/research_report/2116]
-
柏憶涵, Y.-H.,陳嘉玲, C.-L.,張鑑如, C.-J.,陳柏熹, P.-H.,陳協慶, H.-C.,鍾佳英, C.-Y.,汪家琦, C.-C.,傅仁煇, R.-H.,周麗端, L.-T.(2016)。學齡前兒童動作發展評估量表之初步研究:開發與驗證。臺灣復健醫誌,44(2),71-80。
-
張春興, C.-S.,林清山, J.-S.(1998).教育心理學.東華書局=Tung Hua Book.
-
張鑑如 (2019):〈幼兒發展調查資料庫建置計畫:36 月齡組第一波 36 月齡 (D00168)【原始數據】〉。中央研究院學術調查研究資料庫。https://srda.sinica.edu.tw/datasearch_detail.php?id=2953 [Chang, C.-J. (2019). Kids in Taiwan: National Longitudinal Study of Child Development and Care (KIT): KIT-M36 at 36 months old (D00168) [data file]. Survey Research Data Archive, Academia Sinica. https://srda.sinica.edu.tw/datasearch_detail.php?id=2953]
-
陳姣伶, J.-L.,龔如菲, J.-F.(2001).嬰幼兒發展與保育.啟英文化=Qiying.
-
楊子嬿, T.-Y.(2018)。國立臺灣師範大學=National Taiwan Normal University。
-
劉又嘉, Y.-J.(2010)。國立高雄師範大學=National Kaohsiung Normal University。
-
錡寶香, P.-H.(2009).兒童語言與溝通發展.心理出版社=Psychological Publishing.
-
駱明潔, M.-J.,陳依菱, Y.-L.(2012)。中部地區幼兒電視收視行為與健康情形之相關研究。幼兒保育學刊,9,77-99。
-
薛莉慧, L.-H.(2017)。國立臺灣師範大學=National Taiwan Normal University。
-
魏美惠, M.-H.,莊淑媛, S.-Y.(2016)。滑世代幼兒使用 3C 產品及其行為表現與學習態度之研究。資訊科學應用期刊,8(2),47-69。
-
蘇英婷, Y.-T.(2015)。國立臺中教育大學=National Taichung University of Education。
|