.05), which is inconsistent with our first hypothesis. Second, cyberbullying awareness negatively predicted bystanders' reinforcing behavior (adjusted R^2 = .03, F = 40.30, β = -.17, p < .001) and was not moderated by moral disengagement when the victim was cyberbullied without any apparent reason (coefficient = -.02, p = .23 > .05), which is consistent with our second hypothesis. Third, cyberbullying awareness negatively predicted bystanders' defending behavior (adjusted R^2 = .00, F = 4.75, β = -.06, p = .03 < .05) and was not moderated by moral disengagement when the victim was cyberbullied because of his norm-violating behavior (coefficient = .02, p = .29 > .05), which is inconsistent with our third hypothesis. Fourth, cyberbullying awareness negatively predicted bystander's reinforcing behavior (adjusted R^2 = .02, F = 29.57, β = -.14, p < .001) and was moderated by moral disengagement when the victim was cyberbullied because of his norm-violating behavior (coefficient = -.04, p = .03 < .05), which is consistent with our fourth hypothesis. Regardless of whether the victim was cyberbullied for no apparent reason or for violating social norms, the bystanders exhibited only a moderate tendency to defend the victim, and this tendency was not influenced by the degree of cyberbullying awareness. In other words, regardless of whether the bystanders perceived the ongoing event as a cyberbullying incident, their inclination to assist remained the same. According to previous studies (Asanan et al., 2017; Elçi & Seçkin, 2019; Steer et al., 2020), identifying cyberbullying can prevent bystanders from becoming bullies or victims. However, no direct evidence currently exists regarding whether cyberbullying awareness increases the willingness of bystanders to assist. Therefore, cyberbullying awareness is regarded as a prerequisite for initiating defending behavior, with willingness to take action influenced by other factors. In the two scenarios, cyberbullying awareness negatively predicted bystanders' reinforcing behavior. In other words, when the bystanders realized that the current event involved cyberbullying, they became less likely to engage in reinforcing behavior, such as liking, sharing, reposting, or commenting on posts. These findings are consistent with those of Asanan et al. (2017), who indicated that cyberbullying awareness has an inhibitory effect on reinforcing behavior. In this study, we further examined the moderating effect of victim behavior and bystander moral beliefs on the relation between cyberbullying awareness and bystander behavior. Our moderation analysis results indicated that when the victim violated social norms, moral disengagement moderated the relationship between cyberbullying awareness and reinforcing behavior, whereas this was not the case in the regular cyberbullying scenario. These findings support our second and fourth hypotheses, indicating that if bystanders lack cyberbullying awareness and hold beliefs of moral disengagement, such as moral justification, dehumanization, or victim blaming, they may become more inclined to rationalize their reinforcing behavior as an act of justice. Their moral sense may also have a limited influence on their actions, resulting in increased reinforcing behavior. By contrast, if bystanders become actively aware of their own behavior, even if they hold moral disengagement beliefs, they may become less likely to rationalize their reinforcing behavior, which enables them to exercise restraint in their own behavior. On the basis of our research findings, we offer three recommendations for preventing or intervening in cyberbullying incidents. First, because cyberbullying awareness has an inhibitory effect on bystanders, we recommend that schools implement antibullying programs that emphasize cyberbullying awareness among all members of the school community, including teachers, students, and parents. Second, our study indicates that the reactions of bystanders are influenced by the victim's behavior. Bystanders are more likely to refrain from assisting or even engaging in reinforcing behavior if the victim has previously violated social norms. Notably, cyberbullying differs from traditional bullying because online information tends to be one-sided, which can challenge bystanders' ability to fully grasp the situation and the victim, leading to premature judgments regarding the victim's behavior (Liu et al., 2019). Therefore, when bystanders witness a cyberbullying incident, they should attempt to first understand the full context of the event, gather both positive and negative information, and refrain from premature judgments of the victim's behavior. Lastly, in programs aimed at combating cyberbullying, we recommend that teachers incorporate dilemma-based instruction and address the moral disengagement beliefs of their students. For students with high levels of moral disengagement, teachers should explicitly communicate that cyberbullying is an unacceptable means of seeking justice. This study has some limitations. First, our sample comprised only college students, thus limiting the generalizability of our findings to other populations. Second, all data were collected using self-reporting instruments, potentially resulting in monomethod bias. Third, only one norm-violation scenario was examined. Future studies should include a wider range of scenarios to better understand how the perceived severity of cyberbullying incidents influences bystanders' perceptions and actions.' > 华艺学术文献数据库
山西大学,您好!
感谢采购,人社期刊库|人社论文库,期间:2025/01/01~2025/12/31
感谢试用,科学期刊库|科学论文库,期间:2025/04/01~2025/06/30
查询符号(半角) 查询符号意义说明
空格 表示为「AND」两个查询词之交集
双引号 ( " " ) 片语以双引号标示开始及结束,而且只寻找出现顺序相同的字词,例 : " image process "
? 表示一个字母切截,输入两个?表两个字母,依此类推,例:输入「Appl?」,查得结果应为appl e , appl y … ( 常用于英文字查询 )
* 表示不限字母切截,由0~n. 例:输入「appl*」,查得结果应为appl e , appl es , appl y , appl ied , appl ication … ( 常用于英文字查询 )
AND、OR、NOT

布尔逻辑组合关键字,用来扩大或缩小查询范围的技巧。
(1) AND :缩小查询范围
(2) OR :扩大查询范围(3) NOT:排除不相关的范围

close

DOI 是数位物件识别码 ( D igital O bject I dentifier ) 的简称,
为物件在网路上的唯一识别码,可用于永久连结并引用目标物件。

使用DOI作为永久连结

每个DOI号前面加上 「 http://dx.doi.org/ 」 便成为永久网址。
如以DOI号为 10.5297/ser.1201.002 的文献为例,此文献的永久连结便是: http://dx.doi.org/ 10.5297/ser.1201.002
日后不论出版单位如何更动此文献位置,永久连结所指向的位置皆会即时更新,不再错失重要的研究。

引用含有DOI的文献

有DOI的文献在引用时皆应同时引用DOI。若使用APA、Chicago以外未规范DOI的引用格式,可引用DOI永久连结。

DOI可强化引用精确性、增强学术圈连结,并给予使用者跨平台的良好使用经验,目前在全世界已有超过五千万个对象申请DOI。 如想对DOI的使用与概念有进一步了解,请参考 ( ) 。

关闭

ACI:

数据来源:Academic Citation Index,简称ACI

台湾最大的引用文献资料库,目前收录台湾与港澳地区所出版的人文学、社会学领域学术期刊之书目资料与参考文献,总期刊量超过690种,每年定期公布收录期刊的影响指数(Impact Factor)等指标给大众,并提供专家学者与学术单位实用的计量与分析功能。

五年影响指数(5-Year Impact Factor):某一期刊前五年所出版的文章在当年度的平均被引用次数。

公式:(前五年发表论文在统计年的被引用次数)÷(前五年论文产出论文总篇数)

例如:A期刊2017年之五年影响指数

(A期刊2012-2016年发表论文在2017年的被引总次数)/(A期刊2012-2016年发表的论文总数)

关闭

什么是预刊文章?

为提供读者最前线之学术资讯,于期刊文献获同意刊登后、纸本印制完成前,率先于网路线上发表之文章即为预刊文章。预刊文章尚未有卷期、页次及出版日期资讯,但可藉由DOI号识别。 DOI号是文献的数位身份证字号,不论预刊或正式出版皆不会改变,读者可点击DOI连结,或于DOI号前面加上 「 http://dx.doi.org/ 」 连结到文献目前最新版本。

如何引用预刊文章?

请使用预刊文章的线上发表日期及DOI号来引用该篇文献。

引用范例(视不同引文格式规范可能有所差异):

作者姓名。文章篇名。期刊名称。 YYYY/MM/DD线上预先发表。

doi:DOI 号

1 个人觉得 这篇文章 推荐
摘要 〈TOP〉
并列摘要 〈TOP〉
参考文献 ( 74 ) 〈TOP〉
  1. Sung, Y.-H.,Chen, L.-M.(2017).The effectiveness of interventions to improve preservice teachers’ bullying identification.Journal of Educational Practice and Research,30(2),35-66.
    连结:
  2. 李佩珊, P.-S.,谢百淇, P.-P.(2020)。为弱势者发声:高中生之偏见察觉力与旁观者介入知能课程探究。当代教育研究季刊,28(1),39-73。
    连结:
  3. 杜淑芬, S.-F.,邓文章, W.-C.,陈亦凤, Y.-F.(2021)。国中辅导教师介入关系霸凌事件的辅导困境与策略。教育心理学报,53,223-249。
    连结:
  4. 赖怡君, I.-C.,程景琳, C.-L.(2020)。国小学童同理心与霸凌旁观者行为倾向之关系:以道德疏离为中介变项。人类发展与家庭学报,21,28-56。
    连结:
  5. Aboujaoude, E.,Savage, M. W.,Starcevic, V.,Salame, W. O.(2015).Cyberbullying: Review of an old problem gone viral.Journal of Adolescent Health,57(1),10-18.
国际计量 〈TOP〉
E-mail :
文章公开取用时,将寄通知信至您填写的信箱地址
E-mail :

close

close

加入会员完全免费

申请成功后您便可使用 airitiLibrary 所提供的个人化服务!

管理我的最爱期刊清单
管理文章下载记录
管理经常查询的关键字
点数储值优惠
* 用户名 :
请输入经常使用的电子信箱方便记忆
* 密码 :
限定为6-16个半型大小写英文字、数字及底线_为主的符号
* 再次输入密码 :
* 国家 :
备用电子信箱 :
* 验证码 :
我已经了解并同意