3 years. Few studies have investigated how Taiwanese children recognize emotions with incomplete facial information and how they use multiple cues to identify expressions. Current research tools for evaluating children's emotion recognition skills primarily use static images of strangers' faces as stimuli, which fail to capture real-world dynamics. In everyday life, children recognize the emotions of familiar individuals during dynamic interactions by processing facial expressions, body movements, verbal cues (particularly tone of voice), and contextual cues. Standardized emotion recognition tests often feature prototypical emotional expressions displayed by professionals following specific instructions, but these expressions do not reflect the nuances of children's real-life interactions with peers. Therefore, the present study explored children's emotion recognition skills when interacting with masked peers, adapting research tools to better reflect real-life conditions. Instead of relying on picture cards or strangers' photos, this study used videos, rather than static images, of real-life interactions with classmates. This approach facilitated an accurate understanding of children's emotion recognition skills and cue usage when interacting with masked peers. The present study included 151 children aged 3-6 years. V ideos of emotional interactions with peers were used to evaluate children's emotion recognition accuracy and cue usage. The results revealed marked reductions in children's ability to recognize emotions, such as sadness, surprise, fear, and disgust, in masked peers. Thus, children's emotion recognition skills fell short of expectations; they frequently confused emotions within the same category and struggled with classifying basic positive and negative emotions. The analysis of emotional cues indicated that children who accurately recognized emotions used different cues for different emotions. Peers expressed emotions in various ways, which led to variations in emotional cues. Appropriate cues had to be adjusted for emotion recognition. Not all emotions could be identified through a single cue. Children who could not recognize emotions effectively were not only unfamiliar with the definitions of and differences between emotional categories but also struggled to identify appropriate emotional cues. Mask-wearing further reduced their ability to associate cues with the correct emotions, causing confusion even in classifying basic positive and negative emotions. These findings underscore a need for educational interventions to mitigate the effect of mask-wearing on children's emotion recognition skills. Parents and educators should actively teach children strategies for recognizing emotions. Mastering the use of emotion recognition cues can help children accurately recognize the emotions of masked individuals. Thus, children should be taught various strategies and cues for recognizing emotions in daily life and encouraged to observe and interpret the emotional states of masked individuals. Understanding peers' emotions can also enhance children's emotion recognition skills. Thus, peer interaction opportunities should be increased and children should be guided to pay attention to their peers' emotional expressions and feelings. The development of emotion recognition skills from infancy to early childhood involves progression from a basic to more nuanced understanding of emotions, which is vital for social competence and behavioral regulation. However, mask-wearing measures substantially reduced children's exposure to full facial expressions during the pandemic. This reduction was particularly prominent in Taiwan, where such measures were strictly enforced for an extended period. This unique context provided a valuable opportunity for studies to investigate how children adapted to recognizing emotions when the primary cue-facial expression-was partially obscured. Studies from other countries with relatively short pandemic restrictions have begun to address these questions. However, the prolonged mask-wearing measures in Taiwan offers a unique opportunity to explore how children develop emotion recognition skills under sustained conditions of incomplete facial cues. The use of videos of interactions with classmates, rather than static images of strangers, in this study accurately reflected the real-world challenges children face in recognizing emotions during social interactions. This approach also helped leverage children's current relationships with classmates and knowledge of their classmates' typical behaviors and expressions. The approach was based on an acknowledgment that children do not interact with strangers in isolation but rather engage in interactions in dynamic social environments, where they use a combination of verbal and nonverbal cues to interpret emotions. The findings suggested that more flexible and adaptive emotion recognition skills can be developed in children by subjecting them to a broader range of cues beyond facial expressions. Moreover, this study highlighted the importance of contextualizing emotion recognition within the lived experiences of children. Because a major portion of children's early developmental years coincided with the period of mask-wearing measures, understanding how they adapted-or struggled to adapt-to these restrictions is crucial. Such knowledge can inform targeted interventions and educational strategies for enhancing children's emotion recognition skills. Regarding educational recommendations on the basis this study, parents and educators must proactively teach and reinforce emotion recognition strategies-for example, by guiding children to use alternative cues such as body language, vocal tone, and situational context to interpret emotions. Providing opportunities for meaningful peer interactions can further enhance children's ability to recognize and respond to others' emotions. Encouraging children to practice these skills in both masked and unmasked settings can equip them with a comprehensive toolkit for navigating social relationships. In conclusion, this study highlights the profound effect of prolonged mask-wearing measures on the development of children's emotion recognition skills. It calls for collaborative efforts from parents, educators, and researchers to address these challenges and support children in cultivating robust emotion recognition skills that extend beyond the pandemic. The present study clarifies how children adapt to evolving social environments and offers practical guidance for fostering their emotional and social well-being.' > 华艺学术文献数据库
山西大学,您好!
感谢采购,人社期刊库|人社论文库,期间:2025/01/01~2025/12/31
感谢试用,科学期刊库|科学论文库,期间:2025/04/01~2025/06/30
查询符号(半角) 查询符号意义说明
空格 表示为「AND」两个查询词之交集
双引号 ( " " ) 片语以双引号标示开始及结束,而且只寻找出现顺序相同的字词,例 : " image process "
? 表示一个字母切截,输入两个?表两个字母,依此类推,例:输入「Appl?」,查得结果应为appl e , appl y … ( 常用于英文字查询 )
* 表示不限字母切截,由0~n. 例:输入「appl*」,查得结果应为appl e , appl es , appl y , appl ied , appl ication … ( 常用于英文字查询 )
AND、OR、NOT

布尔逻辑组合关键字,用来扩大或缩小查询范围的技巧。
(1) AND :缩小查询范围
(2) OR :扩大查询范围(3) NOT:排除不相关的范围

close

DOI 是数位物件识别码 ( D igital O bject I dentifier ) 的简称,
为物件在网路上的唯一识别码,可用于永久连结并引用目标物件。

使用DOI作为永久连结

每个DOI号前面加上 「 http://dx.doi.org/ 」 便成为永久网址。
如以DOI号为 10.5297/ser.1201.002 的文献为例,此文献的永久连结便是: http://dx.doi.org/ 10.5297/ser.1201.002
日后不论出版单位如何更动此文献位置,永久连结所指向的位置皆会即时更新,不再错失重要的研究。

引用含有DOI的文献

有DOI的文献在引用时皆应同时引用DOI。若使用APA、Chicago以外未规范DOI的引用格式,可引用DOI永久连结。

DOI可强化引用精确性、增强学术圈连结,并给予使用者跨平台的良好使用经验,目前在全世界已有超过五千万个对象申请DOI。 如想对DOI的使用与概念有进一步了解,请参考 ( ) 。

关闭

ACI:

数据来源:Academic Citation Index,简称ACI

台湾最大的引用文献资料库,目前收录台湾与港澳地区所出版的人文学、社会学领域学术期刊之书目资料与参考文献,总期刊量超过690种,每年定期公布收录期刊的影响指数(Impact Factor)等指标给大众,并提供专家学者与学术单位实用的计量与分析功能。

五年影响指数(5-Year Impact Factor):某一期刊前五年所出版的文章在当年度的平均被引用次数。

公式:(前五年发表论文在统计年的被引用次数)÷(前五年论文产出论文总篇数)

例如:A期刊2017年之五年影响指数

(A期刊2012-2016年发表论文在2017年的被引总次数)/(A期刊2012-2016年发表的论文总数)

关闭

什么是预刊文章?

为提供读者最前线之学术资讯,于期刊文献获同意刊登后、纸本印制完成前,率先于网路线上发表之文章即为预刊文章。预刊文章尚未有卷期、页次及出版日期资讯,但可藉由DOI号识别。 DOI号是文献的数位身份证字号,不论预刊或正式出版皆不会改变,读者可点击DOI连结,或于DOI号前面加上 「 http://dx.doi.org/ 」 连结到文献目前最新版本。

如何引用预刊文章?

请使用预刊文章的线上发表日期及DOI号来引用该篇文献。

引用范例(视不同引文格式规范可能有所差异):

作者姓名。文章篇名。期刊名称。 YYYY/MM/DD线上预先发表。

doi:DOI 号

0 个人觉得 这篇文章 推荐
摘要 〈TOP〉
并列摘要 〈TOP〉
参考文献 ( 33 ) 〈TOP〉
  1. 王佩玲, P.-L.(2020)。父母与其五岁子女谈論过去情绪经验用语的差異。教育心理学报,52,51-79。
    连结:
  2. 周育如, Y.-J.,黄乃毓, N.-Y.(2010)。亲子共读情绪言谈与幼儿情绪理解能力之关系检视。教育科学研究期刊,55(3),33-60。
    连结:
  3. 崔新玲, X.-L.,锺志从, J.-T.,梁进龙, J.-L.,吴和堂, H.-T.(2023)。家庭环境影响幼儿语言发展因果关系之研究。教育心理学报,55,1-24。
    连结:
  4. 陈则铭, C.-M,胡中凡, J.-F.,凤华, H.,罗雅芬, Y.(2022)。自闭症谱系障碍儿童在静态情境下情绪辨识表现及眼动研究。特殊教育学报,55,81-115。
    连结:
  5. Barrett, L. F.,Lindquist, K. A.,Gendron, M.(2007).Language as context for the perception of emotion.Trends in Cognitive Sciences,11(8),327-332.
E-mail :
文章公开取用时,将寄通知信至您填写的信箱地址
E-mail :

close

close

加入会员完全免费

申请成功后您便可使用 airitiLibrary 所提供的个人化服务!

管理我的最爱期刊清单
管理文章下载记录
管理经常查询的关键字
点数储值优惠
* 用户名 :
请输入经常使用的电子信箱方便记忆
* 密码 :
限定为6-16个半型大小写英文字、数字及底线_为主的符号
* 再次输入密码 :
* 国家 :
备用电子信箱 :
* 验证码 :
我已经了解并同意