题名

二年級「持續型」與「晚發型」識字困難學童早期區辨效能之檢測

并列篇名

Predicting Persistent or Late-Emerging Difficulties in Reading Chinese Characters in Grade-2 Children

DOI

10.6172/BSE.201407.3902003

作者

宣崇慧(Chung-Hui Hsuan)

关键词

快速自動唸名 ; 注音解碼 ; 區辨函數分析 ; 晚發型識字困難 ; rapid automatized naming (RAN) ; Tzu-yin decoding ; discriminant function analysis ; late-emerging poor reader

期刊名称

特殊教育研究學刊

卷期/出版年月

39卷2期(2014 / 07 / 01)

页次

61 - 86

内容语文

繁體中文

中文摘要

近年來,有許多研究提出學前識字相關認知技能對一年級中文識字困難學童的區辨效能,但亦有研究發現,有些識字困難學童的發生時間約在習字後的二至三年,故稱之為「晚發型」(late-emerging)識字困難學童。因此,本研究縱貫分析學前到小學二年級識字發展相關資料(N=64),以探究早期識字相關技能與其後識字發展的關係,並分析「持續型」(persistent)及「晚發型」識字困難學童之早期識字相關技能的差異,進而檢測早期重要識字技能區辨二年級識字困難學童之效能。結果顯示,學前注音解碼以及快速自動唸名(Rapid Automatized Naming, RAN)與學童一至二年級識字顯著相關,聲韻覺識則隨著識字經驗的增加而愈見重要,視覺記憶的重要性則僅在習字之初。變異數分析結果顯示,在所有變項中,僅注音解碼及RAN達顯著差異,組間比較顯示,「持續型」識字困難學童之注音解碼及RAN顯著低於正常學童;「晚發型」識字困難學童的注音解碼及RAN則介於「持續型」與識字正常學童之間,但並未顯著低於正常學童。區辨分析結果則顯示,早期識字、注音解碼及RAN之組合具最佳區辨效能,分類總正確率達71.9%,其中又以早期識字的區辨效能最佳,亦能有效避免嚴重的假陰型錯誤。一般而言,「持續型」識字困難學童的識字問題較為嚴重,而此等指標對「持續型」識字困難學童的區辨效能相當穩定,但對「晚發型」識字困難學童的區辨效能則較弱。若將「持續型」與「晚發型」兩組合併,本組指標可預測全數「持續型」識字困難學童及80%的「晚發型」識字困難者。

英文摘要

Purpose: Recent studies have proposed that testing for reading-related cognitive skills can identify children with difficulties in reading Chinese characters in Grade 1. Research has also suggested that some children demonstrate adequate reading achievement when they start learning to read but lagmarkedly behind their peers in subsequent grades. These children are referred to as late-emerging poor readers. This study was intended to (1) investigate the importance of a battery of reading-related skills, including phonological awareness in syllable and tone, Tzu-yin decoding, visual memory, and rapid automatized naming (RAN) in character reading from preschool to Grade 2; (2) determine the differences in reading-related skills among groups of children with or without persistent or late-emerging character-reading difficulties; and (3) examine the accuracy of predicting persistent or late-emerging difficulties in reading Chinese characters in children. Methods: This was a 3-year longitudinal study. Although 330 preschoolers were recruited, only 230 remained in the cohort at the end of the study. The participants' reading-related skills were assessed in the last semester of preschool (April to June 2008). Their progress was observed and their reading ability measured at 3 time points: the beginning of Grade 1, the end of Grade 1, and the end of Grade 2. Fifteen of the 230 students had difficultyreading characters in both Grades 1 and 2, and were deemed as having persistent difficulties. Fourteen-children who started having difficulties in Grade 2 were defined as children with late-emerging difficulties. Thirty-five children whose z score of character-reading ranged from 0to 0.5 in both grades were classified as the control group. Correlation, analysis of variances, and discriminant function analysis were employed to analyze the data. Results/ Findings: The results revealed that Tzu-yin decoding and RAN in preschool were relevant to character-reading skills in Grades 1 and 2. The importance of phonological awareness gradually increased with experience in reading, whereas visual memory was substantially associated with character reading only at the initial stage of learning to read. The analysis of variances suggested that Tzu-yin decoding and RAN differed significantly among the groups. Children with persistent difficulties in character-reading scored significantly lower than normal readers in Tzu-yin decoding and RAN. Late-emerging poor readers scored higher thanpersistent poor readers and lower than normal readers in Tzuyin decoding and RAN; however, the differences between late-emerging poor readers and normal readers were not statistically significant. The results of discriminant function analysis provided one crucial discriminant function thatrecognized early character-reading, Tzu-yin decoding, and RAN as significant identifiers of reading difficulties. The classification ratewas 71.9% accurate. Early character-reading was shown to be the most accurate identifier, and offered no false negative values. Conclusions/Implications: The most significant early predictors of reading difficulties in Grade-2 children were Tzu-yin decoding, RAN, and early character-reading. Persistent poor readers might have more difficulties in learning to read than late-emerging poor readers do, and the discriminant function of this study accurately identified persistent poor readers. Althoughthe discriminant functionhad limited success in identifying late-emerging poor readers; the discriminant function predicted 100% of children with persistent difficulties in character reading and 80% of those with late-emerging difficulties from preschool to Grade 2, when the data was analyzed by combining the persistent and late-emerging groups.

主题分类 社會科學 > 教育學
参考文献
  1. 王瓊珠、洪儷瑜、陳秀芬(2007)。低識字能力學生識字量發展之研究:馬太效應之可能表現。特殊教育研究學刊,32(3),1-16。
    連結:
  2. 宣崇慧、盧台華(2006)。聲韻覺識能力及口語詞彙知識與國小一至二年級學童字、詞閱讀發展之關係與影響。特殊教育研究學刊,31,73-92。
    連結:
  3. 宣崇慧、蘇政輝、陳必卿、余孟儒、王涵、張文真、邱郁芬(2012)。學前聲韻處理、快速唸名與視覺記憶能力預測小一學童識字困難效能之檢測。特殊教育研究學刊,37(1),53-78。
    連結:
  4. 黃毅志(2008)。如何精確測量職業地位?「改良版台灣地區新職業聲望與社經地位量表」之建構。台東大學教育學報,19(1),151-160。
    連結:
  5. 教育部統計處(2012):101 學年度各級學校學生人數統計。2013 年 8 月 30 日,取自https://stats.moe.gov.tw/result.aspx?qno=MQA1AA2。[Ministry of Education, Statistics (2013). The population statistics of students at all grades in the 101th academic year. Retrieved August 30, 2013, from https://stats.moe.gov.tw/result.aspx?qno=MQA1AA2.]
  6. 曾世杰(2006):聲調覺識測驗工具說明。2007 年 6 月 30 日,取自 http://www.tald.idv.tw/modules/xfsection/download.php?fileid=21[Tzeng, Shih-Jay (2006). The illustration of phonological awareness test. Retrieved June 30, 2007, from http://www.tald.idv.tw/modules/xfsection/download.php?fileid=21]
  7. 教育部特殊教育通報網(2012):101 學年度一般學校各縣市特教類別學生數統計。2013 年 8 月 30 日,取自 https://www.set.edu.tw/sta2/frame_print.asp?filename=stuA_city_All_spckind_B/stuA_city_All_spckind_B_20130528.asp 。[Ministry of Education, Special Education Transmit Net (2013). The population statistics of students with special needs in classification across all educational categories in the 101th academic year. Retrieved August 30, 2013, from https://www.set.edu.tw/sta2/frame_print.asp?filename=stuA_city_All_spckind_B/stuA_city_All_spckind_B_20130528.asp.]
  8. Catts, H. W.,Compton, D.,Tomblin, J. B.,Bridges, M. S.(2012).Prevalence and nature of late-emerging poor readers.Journal of Educational Psychology,104(1),166-181.
  9. Chall, J. S.(1983).Stages of reading development.New York:McGraw-Hill.
  10. Chan, L.,Nunes, T.(1998).Children's understanding of the formal and functional characteristics of written Chinese.Applied Psycholinguistics,19,115-131.
  11. Compton, D. L.,Fuchs, D.,Fuchs, L. S.,Elleman, A. M.,Gilbert, J. K.(2008).Tracking children who fly below the radar: Latent transition modeling of students with late-emerging reading disability.Learning and Individual Differences,18,329-337.
  12. Ehri, L. C.(1995).Phases of development in learning to read words by sight.Journal of Research in Reading,18(2),116-125.
  13. Gough, P. B.,Tumer, W. E.(1986).Decoding, reading, and reading disability.Remedial and Special Education,7(1),6-10.
  14. Ho, C. S.-H.,Bryant, P.(1997).Phonological skills are important in learning to read Chinese.Developmental Psychology,33,946-951.
  15. Hsuan, C. H.(2010).Longitudinal study of phonological processing, visual memory, RAN, morphological awareness, listening comprehension and reading acquisition upto grade 1.17th Annual conference of Society for Scientific Studies of Reading,Berlin, German:
  16. Huang, H. S.,Hanley, J. R.(1997).A longitudinal study of phonological awareness, visual skills, and Chinese reading acquisition among first-graders in Taiwan.International Journal of Behavioral Development,20(2),249-268.
  17. Lipka, O.,Lesaux, N.,Siegel, L.(2006).Retrospective analyses of the reading development of Grade 4 students with reading disabilities: Risk status and profiles over 5 years.Journal of Learning Disabilities,39,364-378.
  18. McBride-Chang, C.(Ed.),Chen, H.-C.(Ed.)(2003).Reading development in Chinese children.London:Praeger.
  19. McBride-Chang, C.,Ho, C. S.-H.(2005).Predictors of beginning reading in Chinese and English: A 2-year longitudinal study of Chinese kindergarteners.Scientific Studies of Reading,9(2),117-144.
  20. McBride-Chang, C.,Lam, F.,Lam, C.,Chan, B.,Fong, C. Y.-C.,Wong, T. T.-Y.,Wong, S. W.-L.(2011).Early predictors of dyslexia in Chinese children: Family history of dyslexia, language delay, and cognitive profiles.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,52(2),204-211.
  21. O'Connor, R. E.,Jenkins, J. R.(1999).Prediction of reading disabilities in kindergarten and first grade.Scientific Studies of Reading,3(2),159-197.
  22. Pedhazur, E. J.(1982).Multiple regression in behavioral research: Explanation and prediction.New York:Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
  23. Shu, H.,Pen, H.,McBride-Chang, C.(2008).Phonological awareness in young Chinese children.Developmental Science,11(1),171-181.
  24. Siok, W. T.,Fletcher, P.(2001).The role of phonological awareness and visual-orthographic skills in Chinese reading acquisition.Developmental Psychology,37(6),886-899.
  25. Stanovich, K. E.(1986).Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy.Reading Research Quarterly,21(4),360-407.
  26. Tabachnick, B. G.,Fidell, L. S.(2001).Using multivariate statistics.Needham Heights, MA:Allyn & Bacon.
  27. Vellutino, F. R.,Fletcher, J. M.,Snowling, M. J.,Scanlon, D. M.(2004).Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades?.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,45(1),2-40.
  28. Wong, S. W. L.,McBride-Chang, C.,Lam, C.,Chan, B.,Lam, F. W. F.,Doo, S.(2012).The joint effects of risk status, gender, early literacy and cognitive skills on the presence of dyslexia among a group of high-risk Chinese children.Dyslexia,18(1),40-57.
  29. 林彥同(2001)。Kaohsiung, Taiwan,國立高雄師範大學特殊教育研究所=Department of Special Education, National Kaohsiung Normal University。
  30. 宣崇慧(2010)。行政院國家科學委員會專題研究計畫成果報告行政院國家科學委員會專題研究計畫成果報告,行政院國家科學委員會=National Science Council。
  31. 康金雲(2010)。Taichung, Taiwan,國立臺中教育大學教育測驗統計研究所=Graduate Institute of Educational Measurement and Statistics, National Taichung University of Education。
  32. 張毓仁、曾世杰(2008)。國小三年級唸名速度緩慢學童與一般學童閱讀認知能力之比較。教育與心理研究,31(1),179-203。
  33. 陳淑麗、曾世杰(2005)。唸名速度及聲韻覺識在中文閱讀障礙亞型分類上的角色:個案補救教學研究。突破學習困難:評量與因應之探討,臺北=Taipei, Taiwan:
  34. 陳榮華、陳心怡(2003)。魏氏幼兒智力量表修訂版指導手冊。臺北=Taipei, Taiwan:中國行為科學=Chinese Behavioral Science。
  35. 曾世杰、簡淑真、張媛婷、周蘭芳、連芸伶(2005)。以早期唸名速度及聲韻覺識預測中文閱讀與認字:一個追蹤四年的相關研究。特殊教育研究學刊,28,123-144。
  36. 黃沛榮(2001)。漢字教學的理論與實踐。臺北=Taipei, Taiwan:樂學=Lexis Book。
  37. 黃秀霜(2001)。中文年級識字測驗。臺北=Taipei, Taiwan:心理=Psychological。
被引用次数
  1. 蔡建鈞、宣崇慧(2016)。學前識字困難高危險群幼兒之鑑別:學前教師評定搭配認知測驗兩階段篩選機制區辨效能之檢驗。特殊教育研究學刊,41(2),27-56。
  2. 陳志平、張淑蘋、范姜雅菁、孟瑛如(2015)。國民小學4-6年級學童識字診斷測驗編製與探究。特教論壇,19,69-84。
  3. 林振興、王暄博(2017)。華語學習者漢字能力與聲韻覺識、視知覺能力之相關研究。華語文教學研究,14(4),71-100。
  4. 盧玉真、陳志平、孟瑛如(2016)。「國民中學七至九年級識字診斷測驗」編製研究。測驗學刊,63(3),203-226。
  5. 周姍姍,王馨敏(2021)。曾世杰教授專題演講「教育能造成改變:以弱勢兒童低學力問題為例」學術活動紀實。人類發展與家庭學報,22,112-119。